Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Phonological Analysis of Dinka Language: Number Marking in Nouns, Exams of Voice

An in-depth analysis of the phonological features of the Dinka language, focusing on number marking in nouns. the tripartite number marking system, which includes differences in vowel length, tone, and voice quality. Examples of singular and plural forms of various Dinka nouns are presented, illustrating the patterns of number marking. The document also compares the number marking in Dinka to that of Anywa and Luanyjang Dinka, providing valuable insights for linguistic research and language learners.

Typology: Exams

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/27/2022

mdbovary
mdbovary 🇬🇧

4.8

(8)

215 documents

1 / 13

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
On the distinction between regular and irregular inflectional morphology:
evidence from Dinka
D. Robert Ladd, Bert Remijsen, Caguor Adong Manyang*
University of Edinburgh and *University of Bahr el-Ghazal
To appear as a Short Research Report in Language
Abstract
Discussions of the psycholinguistic significance of regularity in inflectional morphology
generally deal with languages in which regular forms can be clearly identified and revolve
around whether there are distinct processing mechanisms for regular and irregular forms. We
present a detailed description of Dinka’s notoriously irregular noun number inflection and
suggest that no pattern can usefully be designated as regular. Psycholinguistic studies of
Dinka would make a valuable contribution to our understanding of inflectional morphology
cross-linguistically.*
*
This research was sponsored primarily by the U.K. Arts and Humanities Research Council
(Research Grant no. MRG-AN11781/APN19394 to Edinburgh University). The involvement
of Caguor Adong Manyang was made possible by a Small Research Grant (no. 39265) from
the British Academy. The research also forms part of Ladd’s project ‘Simultaneous and
sequential structure in language’, supported by a Leverhulme Individual Research Fellowship.
We gratefully acknowledge this support. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the
15th Manchester Phonology Meeting in 2007.
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8
pf9
pfa
pfd

Partial preview of the text

Download Phonological Analysis of Dinka Language: Number Marking in Nouns and more Exams Voice in PDF only on Docsity!

On the distinction between regular and irregular inflectional morphology: evidence from Dinka

D. Robert Ladd, Bert Remijsen, Caguor Adong Manyang* University of Edinburgh and *University of Bahr el-Ghazal

To appear as a Short Research Report in Language

Abstract

Discussions of the psycholinguistic significance of regularity in inflectional morphology generally deal with languages in which regular forms can be clearly identified and revolve around whether there are distinct processing mechanisms for regular and irregular forms. We present a detailed description of Dinka’s notoriously irregular noun number inflection and suggest that no pattern can usefully be designated as regular. Psycholinguistic studies of Dinka would make a valuable contribution to our understanding of inflectional morphology cross-linguistically.*

  • (^) This research was sponsored primarily by the U.K. Arts and Humanities Research Council

(Research Grant no. MRG-AN11781/APN19394 to Edinburgh University). The involvement of Caguor Adong Manyang was made possible by a Small Research Grant (no. 39265) from the British Academy. The research also forms part of Ladd’s project ‘Simultaneous and sequential structure in language’, supported by a Leverhulme Individual Research Fellowship. We gratefully acknowledge this support. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 15th Manchester Phonology Meeting in 2007.

  1. Introduction.

Discussion of the acquisition and processing of inflectional morphology has for some time been dominated by a debate over the relation between regular and irregular forms. According to the ‘dual-route’ hypothesis (e.g. Pinker 1999, Pinker and Ullman 2002), there are two separate processing mechanisms, one based on the productive application of rules and the other based on lookup of stored forms in the mental lexicon. There are stronger and weaker versions of this view, but all presuppose that regular forms are normally parsed into, and assembled from, their component parts, while irregular forms are normally stored whole in the lexicon. The opposing view, based originally on connectionist modeling (beginning with Rumelhart and McClelland 1986; for more current reviews see McClelland and Patterson 2002 and Baayen 2007), holds that all morphological acquisition and processing is based on the development of associations between morphologically related forms, and that the strength of the associations depends on a large number of factors, including the frequency of the forms and the frequency of the patterns involved. In between these two extremes lie a range of empirical studies of morphological acquisition and processing, which argue in various ways that regularity is a matter of degree. For example, the work of Ravid and her colleagues (e.g. Laaha et al. 2006 on noun plurals in German, Ravid and Schiff 2009 on noun plurals in Hebrew) is based on a two-dimensional characterization of regularity, involving degrees of ‘suffix regularity’ and ‘stem change’, and shows that such a description is consistent with many findings about how morphology is acquired. Albright’s work (2002) on sub-regularities in Italian verb morphology suggests that there are ‘islands of reliability’ within the set of irregular forms in any given system, and that speakers are able to make productive use of these sub-regularities.

What all this work shares, however – in addition to being based largely on well-studied languages – is the assumption that it is possible to distinguish regular and irregular (or more regular and less regular) inflection. Even work that explicitly argues for a gradient conception of regularity (like that of Ravid and her colleagues), or connectionist work that seeks to explain regularity without any special mechanisms, takes the notion of regularity itself for granted. In this squib we present data from the noun number marking system of Dinka, which make it appear entirely possible for a rich inflectional system not to have any patterns that can be identified as regular at all. We briefly consider the potential implications of this for theories of morphological acquisition and processing.

1.1. Background information on Dinka.

Dinka is a Western Nilotic language within the Nilo-Saharan family, spoken by more than two million speakers (Gordon 2005). The Dinka homeland is along the banks of the White Nile and its tributaries in Southern Sudan, but there are now large communities in Khartoum and in other countries as well. A description of the Dinka sound system (Agar dialect) can be found in Andersen (1987). Dinka is of particular phonological interest because of its rich suprasegmental system, which includes independent distinctions of tone, vowel length and voice quality. The dialect represented here (Luanyjang, part of the Rek dialect cluster) has four tonemes (Remijsen and Ladd 2008), three degrees of vowel length (Remijsen and Gilley 2008), and a two-way distinction of voice quality (breathy vs. modal or creaky) (Remijsen and Manyang 2009); the details of other dialects (many of which are essentially undocumented) differ in detail but are broadly similar.

referring to things that are typically encountered in masses, sets or pairs (e.g. grass, ants, fingers, or eyes) are often unmarked in the plural, and in many Nilo-Saharan languages are marked by an affix in the singular. Nouns referring to things that are typically encountered as countable individuals (e.g. chief, river, cattle camp) are often unmarked in the singular, and appear with an affix in the plural. The semantic basis of the system is thus related to the distinction between mass and count nouns found in many other languages. However, as with noun class systems everywhere, it is not always easy to detect a semantic basis for the morphological treatment of any given noun; moreover, there is a third group of nouns that has affixes for number in both the singular and the plural. This ‘tripartite’ number marking system is found across the Nilo-Saharan language family (Dimmendaal 2000); this is illustrated in 2 with examples from Anywa taken from Reh (1999), cited in Storch (2005:226ff):

(2) Tripartite number marking in Anywa

Singular Plural Gloss

cn-  c ‘hand’ (marked singular)

c idi ‘belly’ (marked plural)

tuu-o tuu-e ‘palm tree’ (both singular and plural marked)

In Dinka, number inflection involves the tripartite system just outlined to at least some extent; in addition, nouns are inflected for locative case, and to signal the presence of a modifier (Andersen 2002). As explained in the previous section, however, Dinka has lost most of its suffixes, and number marking that is signaled by an affix in a typical Nilo-Saharan language often involves a longer vowel in Dinka. The corresponding words in Dinka to the Anywa examples just given are shown in 3:

(3) Tripartite number marking in Dinka

Singular Plural Gloss

ciin n ‘hand’ (marked singular)

jc jeeec ‘belly’ (marked plural)

tuuk tuuk ‘fruit of palm’ (both singular and plural marked)

More generally, number marking in Dinka involves the same stem-internal processes found in the verb morphology, namely changes in tone, vowel quality, voice quality and the nature of the coda consonant. Unlike the verb morphology, though, where the patterns of modification are quite fixed (as in the example of 1st^ singular formation above), no obvious generalizations hold for number marking on nouns. The only generalization with any degree of validity is the one involving vowel length just described, which seems to hold across a substantial proportion of nouns.^2 However, none of the other stem-internal phonological dimensions involved in marking number appear to show any such predictability.

The range of variation is illustrated in 4. Number can be marked by the change of a single phonological parameter, e.g. tone in 4a or vowel length in 4b. In 4c,d, the difference between singular and plural is marked by changes in both vowel length and tone – but different

(^2) It is important, though, not to exaggerate the extent of the semantic regularity. Among animal names, for

example, there are cases in which names of animals typically encountered in herds or groups show a longer vowel in the plural (e.g. /kw / k‘[kind of] gazelle SG/PL’) and names of typically solitary animals show a longer vowel in the singular (e.g. /kjt / kít/ ‘scorpion SG/PL’).

changes in the two cases. In 4e,f, three parameters are involved – tone, vowel height, and breaking or diphthongization in 4e, and tone, vowel length and voice quality in 4f. Finally, the examples in 4g,h illustrate cases involving four changes: vowel length, tone, vowel height, and voice quality in 4g, and tone, vowel height, voice quality and the coda consonant in 4h.

(4) Singular Plural Gloss

a. piic piic ‘stirring stick’ b. bi biii ‘cup’ c. ba baa ‘chief’ d. nooon noon ‘grass’ e. tiil tjl ‘thistle’ f. waal wal ‘plant’ g. lwak lweek ‘cattle byre’ h. r  w root ‘hippo’

The examples in 4 are not isolated exceptions, but are typical of Dinka number inflection, suggesting that there may be no regular way to mark number in Dinka at all – in other words, no group or class of nouns that can be treated as ‘regular’. This squib reports a detailed investigation of this possibility. We aimed to answer two specific questions: First, what combinations of phonological parameters are involved in the marking of number in Dinka nouns, and how many such combinations are there? Second, are there any obvious sub- regularities or conditioning factors determining which combination is used in any given case, and is there any evidence for a default choice?

  1. The study.

2.1. Dataset.

For 400 Dinka nouns, we collected both the singular and the plural forms. This is not an entirely trivial task, in part because of the phonetic nature of the distinctions involved. The three-way length distinction can be difficult to hear in isolation; one of the tonal distinctions is neutralized or nearly neutralized in citation form (Remijsen and Ladd 2008); there are allophonic interactions of voice quality and vowel quality (Remijsen and Manyang 2009); and in the still-developing orthography the suprasegmental distinctions are represented inadequately or not at all. Consequently we took great care to be sure that the forms we report are correct, putting words in different sentence contexts and carrying out instrumental acoustic measurements where necessary, and we are confident that the data can be considered reliable. As noted above, the dialect under study is Luanyjang Dinka, the sound system of which is familiar to us from several studies carried out in conjunction with the work reported here (Remijsen and Gilley 2008; Remijsen and Ladd 2008; Remijsen and Manyang 2009). The data are based on the speech of the third author, but have been checked for consistency with other speakers in Khartoum; for only 10 nouns out of the 400 was there any variability.

The set of nouns investigated includes (a) 312 native monosyllabic words^3 , which are characteristic of the great majority of the Dinka lexicon; (b) 61 native polysyllabic but

(^3) More accurately, the native monosyllables under investigation include 312 singular-plural pairs. The sample

contains eight singulars (words meaning ‘child’, ‘boy’, ‘girl’, ‘woman’, ‘man’, ‘cow’, ‘bull’, and ‘goat’) that have two semantically distinct plurals, one referring to countable individuals (here abbreviated PL) and one referring to a collective (here abbreviated UNC); these were counted as 16 distinct singular-plural pairs rather than eight lexical items.

2.2. Native monosyllabic nouns.

Here we consider 307 native monosyllabic nouns.^8 Within this set, we found 81 different combinations of the above-mentioned parameters. There is great variation in the extent to which the various parameters are involved: a difference in Tone is involved in the marking of number for 85% of these nouns, whereas vowel Backness differs only for 1%. The values for the other parameters lie between these extremes: 71% for vowel Length; 49% for vowel Height, 21% for vowel Breaking, and 11% for both Voice quality and Coda consonant. The ten most frequent combinations of parameter differences are shown in Table 2.

  1. Length (1), Tone (L~H) 12.0% (37)
  2. Tone (L~H) 6.8% (21)
  3. Length (1), Tone (L~LH) 6.5% (20)
  4. Height, Length (1), Tone (L~H), Breaking 6.2% (19)
  5. Length (1), Tone (L~HL) 5.2% (16)
  6. Height, Length (1), Tone (L~H) 4.2% (13)
  7. Height 3.6% (11)
  8. Height, Tone (L~H), Breaking 3.2% (10)
  9. Height, Length (1), Tone (L~LH) 3.2% (10)
  10. Length (1), Tone (H~LH) 2.9% (9) Table 2: The most common patterns of number marking for native monosyllabic nouns in Luanyjang Dinka, with percentage (%) and nunber of cases (in brackets).

As seen from Table 2, combinations of Length, Tone and Height differences are particularly common. These ten most frequent combinations account for 57% of the set of native monosyllabic nouns. The patterns involved in the marking of number in the remainder of these nouns are represented by between 1 and 8 cases.

2.3. Native polysyllabic nouns.

Among native stems, the pattern /a(CV)CV(V)(V)C/ is the only polysyllabic noun template (cf. Andersen 1987, Storch 2005:168). The initial vowel may have a derivational origin, but synchronically it is part of the stem, and the words can be considered monomorphemic. Tone is specified only on the final syllable (Remijsen and Ladd 2008: 195-196) and only the final syllable is involved in stem-internal inflectional changes. Three examples are presented in 6.

(6) Singular Plural

a. a wool a ol ‘maize’ b. a ikool a ikoool ‘story’ c. atjeep atiiip ‘shelter’

Our dataset includes 61 such nouns. Within this set, there are 33 combinations of phonological parameter differences. All but one of these 33 combinations also occurred in the

(^8) Of the original set of 312, we exclude from consideration five that may be said to involve suppletion, in which

singular and plural differ in onset consonant as well as in other ways, namely /tiik/ vs. /djaaar/ ‘woman, wife:SG/PL’, /tiik/ vs. /djooor/ ‘woman, wife:SG/UNC’, /wee / vs. /ok/ ‘cow:SG/PL’, /raaan/ vs. /kc/ ‘person:SG/PL’ and /mooc/ vs. /ro oor/ ‘man: SG/PL’. For the two plural forms of ‘woman, wife’ see footnote 3.

set of native monosyllabic set of nouns. The ten most frequent combinations of parameters are listed in Table 3.

  1. Length (1), Tone (L~H) 9.8% (6)
  2. Length (1), Tone (L~LH) 8.2% (5)
  3. Length (1), Tone (H~LH) 8.2% (5)
  4. Length (1), Tone (L~HL) 6.6% (4)
  5. Length (1), Tone (LH~HL) 6.6% (4)
  6. Length (2), Tone (L~H) 4.9% (3)
  7. Height, Length (1), Tone (L~H) 4.9% (3)
  8. Length (1) 3.3% (2)
  9. Length (1), Tone (H~HL) 3.3% (2)
  10. Length (2), Tone (L~HL) 3.3% (2)

Table 3:The most common patterns of number marking for native polysyllabic nouns in Luanyjang Dinka, with percentage (%) and number of cases (in brackets).

These ten patterns cover 59% of the native polysyllabic nouns. Impressionistically comparing this top-ten with the one for monosyllabic nouns, we find that combinations of vowel Length and Tone are more prevalent among the set of polysyllabic nouns, suggesting that vowel Height plays less of a role here than in the monosyllabic set. Indeed, a count reveals that differences of vowel Height are involved in number marking in only 39% of the polysyllabic nouns, as compared to 49% for the monosyllables. In other respects the monosyllabic and polysyllabic nouns seem to behave similarly.

2.4. Sub-regularities in native monomorphemic noun number marking.

The fact that vowel Height differences are used in number marking to a greater extent in monosyllables than in polysyllables could be seen as a sub-regularity in the number marking system, of the sort investigated for Italian verb conjugations by Albright (2002). That is, knowing that vowel Height differences are less common in native polysyllabic nouns could provide at least probabilistic support for the learner of Dinka in reliably storing singular-plural pairs. In this section we summarise our observations on such sub-regularities and ‘islands of reliability’ seen in the parts of our sample discussed so far. There clearly are some such generalizations, especially with regard to Tone, though none of them go beyond probabilistic statements, and many are restricted to very specific subsets of the corpus. Except where explicitly noted, however, all these generalizations appear to apply similarly in both monosyllables and polysyllables.

i. With regard to Length, roughly 60% of nouns differing by one degree of Length ( cases) involve a medium (/VV/) and a long (/VVV/) vowel, while only 40% involve a short (/V/) and a medium (/VV/) vowel. The overwhelming majority (>80%) of nouns that do not differ in Length (99 cases) are medium (/VV/) in both singular and plural, and are almost never short (/V/) (only 3 cases). Where differences of Length are involved in number marking, they are about equally likely to involve lengthening from singular to plural as from plural to singular (cf. the comments on number marking in section 1.2). ii. With regard to Height, nouns are less likely to have number marked by a difference of Height if it is also marked by a difference of Length. Some 65% of singular-plural pairs that do not differ in Length differ in Height, whereas only 40% of pairs that differ in Length also differ in Height. Where there are differences of Height, they are

Cattle nouns are derived from attribute terms that denote the colours or patterns of hides; for bulls, the nominalization involves the prefix /ma-/mi-/ (SG/PL). (It may be relevant to point out that traditional Dinka economy, society and folklore are all heavily based on the keeping of cattle.) The four examples in our corpus are presented in 7.

(7) Singular Plural

a. ma-kwac mi-kwec ‘spotted bull’ b. ma-j  k mi-j  k ‘black bull with white chest’ c. ma-k  l mi-k  l ‘red-brown and white patterned bull’ d. ma-caaar mi-c  r ‘black bull’

As seen from these examples, the marking of number in cattle nouns involves the same mechanisms familiar from the basic nouns, such as differences of Tone, Length, and vowel Height. There are too few cases to determine whether there might be sub-regularities within the set.

The second group of nominalizations are formed from adjective/verb stems by the addition of the prefix /ke-/ka-/ (SG/PL), yielding an often abstract noun. The six examples in our corpus

are shown in 8.

(8) Singular Plural

ke-pjat ka-pjat ‘something good’ (< pjat ‘good’)

ke-cool ka-col ‘something black’

ke-reem ka-reem ‘pain’

ke-wen ka-wen ‘something old’

ke-wiiir ka-wiiir ‘water spirit’

ke-raac ka-rac ‘snake’ (< ràc ‘bad’)

The plural formations here are largely regular, though complex, and quite specific to this set of nouns. The prefix has a singular form /ke-/ and a plural form that varies between /ka-/ and

/ka-/ according to whether the following stem has breathy voice quality or not. The stems themselves show two different patterns of behavior: some are invariable, while others vary in vowel length, normally longer in the singular and shorter in the plural. This pattern of length variation is seen in many free adjective forms when they are in construction with a noun (e.g. /tiim cool/ ‘black tree’ vs. /tiiim col/ ‘black trees’).

The third group of nominalizations consists of agent nouns formed from verbs or other noun stems by the addition of the prefix /mi-/. Our five examples are shown in 9.

(9) Singular Plural

a. mi-kook mi-koook ‘greedy person’ b. mi- aal mi- aaal ‘father of girls’ c. mi-k  r mi-k  r ‘adulterer’ d. mi-kwiii mi-kwiii ‘miser’ e. mi-wat mi-waaat ‘father of boys’

It can be seen that these all follow generalizations (iv) and (vi) in section 2.4, namely that at least one of the two forms in each pair involves L tone and that the L tone goes with the form

that has the longer vowel if a difference in vowel Length is involved. More specifically, in fact, in four of the five cases we have a plural form with a long (/VVV/) vowel and a L tone. Whether by coincidence or not, this combination reappears in most other reduplicated forms and in nine of the ten loan words. The reduplicated forms in 10 and the loan words exemplified in 11 show that most of the polysyllabic words in these groups have the plural form with a long (/VVV/) vowel and L tone.

(10) Singular Plural Gloss a. ma-circir ma-circiiir ‘bicycle’ b. ma-tuktuuk ma-tuktuuuk ‘car’ (11) Singular Plural Gloss a. tukuul tukuuul ‘school (< Eng.)’ b. galam galaaam ‘pen (< Ar. galam-aglaam)’ c. garneet garneeet ‘grenade (< Eng.)’ d. kumbaaj kumbaaaj ‘cup (< Ar. kubbaaya)’

e. taaan tn ‘plate (< Ar. s  aHan-s  oHoon]’

The only exception to this pattern among the loanwords is the monosyllabic 11e, which has a relatively rare combination of parameter differences typical of the native monosyllables. The other words in 10 and 11 are polysyllabic, and deviate phonologically from the segmental template for native polysyllables. It may be that, as a group, these words, together with the agent nouns, have developed or are developing their own sub-regularity (long (/VVV/) vowel plus L tone in the plural) for number marking. This sub-regularity would have a status in Dinka comparable to the use of the /-s/ plural suffix in German, which has been discussed by various authors. But even here, two out of our 18 cases constitute exceptions to this pattern, and we note that loanwords are traditionally rather rare in Dinka and may still be disfavored.

  1. Discussion and conclusion.

Our dataset of 400 Dinka nouns yields 82 combinations of phonological differences that can be used to mark the distinction between singular and plural. Some patterns appear with greater frequency than others: that is, patterns involving some combination of differences in Tone, vowel Length, and vowel Height are more common than other combinations. However, though there are clearly some tendencies and probabilistic generalizations about how the phonological differences can be combined, it does not appear possible to identify any phonological or semantic motivation for the choice of number marking pattern for a given noun. Only in some of the derived nominals and loanwords do we see something like a pattern that we might consider to be regular: in the deverbal/deadjectival nouns we see a complex but consistent pattern involving separate singular and plural prefixes, while in agent nouns, reduplicated forms and loanwords we find that most plural forms have a long (/VVV/) vowel and L tone.

Similar degrees of irregularity have been reported for other Western Nilotic languages (e.g. Welmers 1973, Frank 1999, Gilley 2000, Storch 2005). The best-studied case in this context is Nuer, which together with Atuot is the language most closely related to Dinka. Here, as in Dinka, stem-internal changes predominate in the marking of number, but, unlike Dinka, Nuer also has a suffixal marking, which shows up in about 20% of nouns (Frank 1999:9-10). Studies on Nuer noun morphology similar to our own have been reported in Welmers (1973) and Frank (1999). Both report finding several dozen patterns of inflectional marking,

Andersen, Torben. 2002. Case inflection and nominal head marking in Dinka. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 23: 1-30.

Baayen, R. Harald. 2007. Storage and computation in the mental lexicon. In G. Jarema and G. Libben (eds.). The Mental Lexicon: Core Perspectives. Elsevier, 81-104.

Bybee, Joan L. and Jean E. Newman. 1995. Are stem changes as natural as affixes? Linguistics 33: 633-654.

Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. 2000. Number Marking and Noun Categorization in Nilo-Saharan Languages. Anthropological Linguistics 42: 214-261.

Frank, Wright Jay. 1999. Nuer noun morphology. MA dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo.

Gilley, Leoma G. 2000. Singulars and Plurals in Shilluk – a search for order. Occasional Papers in the Study of Sudanese Languages 8.

Gleason, Jean Berko. 1958. The child's learning of English morphology. Word 14:150-77.

Laaha, Sabine, Dorit Ravid, Katharina Korecky-Kröll, Gregor Laaha and Wolfgang U. Dressler. 2006. Early noun plurals in German: regularity, productivity or default? Journal of Child Language 33: 271-302.

McClelland, James, and Karalyn Patterson. 2002. Words or rules cannot exploit the regularity in exceptions: reply to Pinker and Ullman. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 465-472.

Pinker, Steven. 1999. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York: Basic Books.

Pinker, Steven, and Michael Ullman. 2002. The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 456-463.

Ravid, Dorit and Rachel Schiff. 2009. Morphophonological categories of noun plurals in Hebrew: a developmental study. Linguistics 47: 45-63.

Reh, Mechthild. 1999. Anywa-English and English-Anywa Dictionary. Cologne: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.

Remijsen, Bert and Caguor Adong Manyang. 2009. Luanyjang Dinka. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 39: 113-124.

Remijsen, Bert and Leoma G. Gilley. 2008. Why are three-level vowel length systems rare? Insights from Dinka (Luanyjang dialect). Journal of Phonetics 36: 318-344.

Remijsen, Bert and D. Robert Ladd. 2008. The tone system of the Luanyjang dialect of Dinka. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 29: 173-213.

Rumelhart, David, and James McClelland. 1986. On learning the past tense of English verbs. In J. McClelland, D. Rumelhart, and the PDP Research Group (eds.), Parallel Distributed Processing, vol. 2, pp. 216-271. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Storch, Anne. 2005. The Noun Morphology of Western Nilotic. Nilo-Saharan vol. 21. Cologne: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.

Welmers, William. 1973. African Language Structures. Berkeley: University of California Press.