Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Auditory Neglect: A Study on Patients with Hemispheric Lesions, Lecture notes of Neurology

A research article published in the Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry in 1989. The study investigates auditory neglect in normal controls and patients with unilateral hemispheric lesions. The authors required participants to detect interruptions in one ear using mono-aural and binaural presentations. The study found that control patients accurately detected interruptions, while patients with lesions in the parietal lobe or thalamus exhibited auditory neglect. The relationship between auditory neglect and auditory extinction was also investigated, and the findings were equivocal. The document also discusses the correlation between visual and auditory neglect and the participation of the right hemisphere in deploying attention to both contralateral and ipsilateral spaces.

Typology: Lecture notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/12/2022

unknown user
unknown user 🇬🇧

1 / 5

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
Journal
of
Neurology,
Neurosurgery,
and
Psychiatry
1989;52:613-617
Auditory
neglect
E
DE
RENZI,
M
GENTILINI,
C
BARBIERI
From
the
Neurological
Department,
Modena
University,
Modena,
Italy
SUMMARY
Auditory
neglect
was
investigated
in
normal
controls
and
in
patients
with
a
recent
unilateral
hemispheric
lesion,
by
requiring
them
to
detect
the
interruptions
that
occurred
in
one
ear
in
a
sound
delivered
through
earphones
either
mono-aurally
or
binaurally.
Control
patients
accurately
detected
interruptions.
One
left
brain
damaged
(LBD)
patient
missed
only
once
in
the
ipsilateral
ear
while
seven
of
the
30
right
brain
damaged
(RBD)
patients
missed
more
than
one
signal
in
the
mono-
aural
test
and
nine
patients
did
the
same
in
the
binaural
test.
Omissions
were
always
more
marked
in
the
left
ear
and
in
the
binaural
test
with
a
significant
ear
by
test
interaction.
The
lesion
of
these
patients
was
in
the
parietal
lobe
(five
patients)
and
the
thalamus
(four
patients).
The
relation
of
auditory
neglect
to
auditory
extinction
was
investigated
and
found
to
be
equivocal,
in
that
there
were
seven
RBD
patients
who
showed
extinction,
but
not
neglect
and,
more
importantly,
two
patients
who
exhibited
the
opposite
pattern,
thus
challenging
the
view
that
extinction
is
a
minor
form
of
neglect.
Also
visual
and
auditory
neglect
were
not
consistently
correlated,
the
former
being
present
in
nine
RBD
patients
without
auditory
neglect
and
the
latter
in
two
RBD
patients
without
visual
neglect.
The
finding
that
in
some
RBD
patients
with
auditory
neglect
omissions
also
occurred,
though
with
less
frequency,
in
the
right
ear,
points
to
a
right
hemisphere
participation
in
the
deployment
of
attention
not
only
to
the
contralateral,
but
also
to
the
ipsilateral
space.
Neglect
phenomena
following
right
brain
damage
have
been
extensively
investigated
in
the
visual
modality,
where
they
manifest
themselves
as
failure
to
orient
attention
to
left-sided
stimuli,
which
may
go
undetected
under
conditions
that
should
urge
the
patient
to
survey
the
whole
space
(for
example,
when
required
to
read
a
sentence,
which
lacks
meaning
if
only
the
words
lying
to
the
right
are
taken
into
account).
While
visual
neglect
is
readily
apparent
on
a
series
of
tasks
requiring
the
scanning
of
the
environ-
ment,
no
convincing
evidence
of
failure
to
respond
to
auditory
stimuli
having
their
source
located
in
the
left
space
has
so
far
been
provided.
Right
brain
damaged
patients
do
show
errors
of
localisation
and
lateralisa-
tion
of
auditory
stimuli,
shifting
them
towards
the
right
side'2
and
sometimes
answer
a
question
ad-
dressed
from
the
left
turning
their
head
and
eyes
to
the
right,3
but
they
have
never
been
reported
to
ignore
a
stimulus
located
in
the
left
space,
except
in
response
to
double
simultaneous
stimulation.
Extinction,
that
is,
the
failure
to
perceive
a
stimulus
in
the
contralateral
Address
for
reprint
requests:
Dr
Ennio
De
Renzi,
Clinica
Neurologica,
Via
Del
Pozzo
71,
1-41100
Modena,
Italy.
Received
10
May
1988
and
in
revised
form
3
December
1988.
Accepted
12
December
1988
ear
when
it
is
simultaneously
delivered
to
the
ipsilateral
ear,
has
been
observed
in
nearly
50%
of
unilaterally
brain
damaged
patients
in
the
early
stage
of
a
stroke,4
but,
contrary
to
what
happens
with
visual
neglect,
no
significant
association
of
the
deficit
with
right
hemisphere
lesions
has
been
observed.
This
lack
of
asymmetry
in
auditory
extinction,
which
has
been
confirmed
in
a
recent
investigation,5
casts
some
doubt
on
the
close
relation
of
extinction
to
neglect.
Moreover,
there
were
patients
with
long
lasting
extinc-
tion,4
whose
injury
was
so
located
as
to
interrupt
the
central
auditory
pathways
in
their
course
from
the
medial
geniculate
body
to
the
temporal
cortex,
thus
making
extinction
more
likely
to
be
contingent
on
a
sensory
than
an
attentional
impairment.
The
difficulty
of
showing
neglect
in
the
auditory
modality
is
probably
due
to
the
physical
characteris-
tics
of
acoustic
stimuli
and
the
arrangement
of
the
neural
apparatus
transmitting
them
to
the
cortical
centres.
The
sound
waves
emitted
by
a
left
sided
acoustic
source
reach
both
ears,
though
for
the
right
one
with
delay
and
decreased
intensity,6
and
are
thence
sent
to
both
temporal
cortices,
though
the
contra-
lateral
pathway
is
stronger.
It
follows
that,
when
the
right
hemisphere
is
damaged,
the
left
hemisphere
remains
able
to
perceive
and
attend
to
a
sound
coming
from
the
left
side,
due
to
its
connections
with
both
613
pf3
pf4
pf5

Partial preview of the text

Download Auditory Neglect: A Study on Patients with Hemispheric Lesions and more Lecture notes Neurology in PDF only on Docsity!

Journal ofNeurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 1989;52:613-

Auditory neglect

E DE RENZI, M GENTILINI, C BARBIERI

From the Neurological Department, Modena University, Modena, Italy

SUMMARY Auditory neglect was investigated in (^) normal controls and in patients with a recent unilateral hemispheric lesion, by requiring them (^) to detect the interruptions that occurred in one ear in a sound delivered through earphones either mono-aurally or binaurally. Control patients accurately detected interruptions. One left brain damaged (LBD) (^) patient missed only once in the ipsilateral ear while seven of the 30 right brain damaged (RBD) patients missed more than one (^) signal in the mono- aural test and nine patients did the same in the binaural (^) test. Omissions were always more marked in the left ear and in the binaural test with a significant ear by test (^) interaction. The lesion ofthese patients

was in the parietal lobe (five patients) and the thalamus (four patients). The relation of auditory

neglect to auditory extinction was investigated and found to be equivocal, in that (^) there were seven

RBD patients who showed extinction, but not neglect and, more importantly, two patients who

exhibited the opposite pattern, thus challenging the view that extinction is a minor form of neglect.

Also visual and auditory neglect were not consistently correlated, the former being present in nine

RBD patients without auditory neglect and the latter in two RBD patients without visual neglect. The finding that in some RBD patients with auditory neglect omissions also occurred, though with less frequency, in the right ear, points to a right hemisphere participation in the deployment of attention not only to the contralateral, but also to the ipsilateral space.

Neglect phenomena following right brain damage

have been extensively investigated in the visual

modality, where they manifest themselves as failure to

orient attention to left-sided stimuli, which may go

undetected under conditions that should urge the

patient to survey the whole space (for example, when

required to read a sentence, which lacks (^) meaning if

only the words lying to the right are taken into

account). While visual neglect is readily apparent on a

series of tasks requiring the scanning of the environ-

ment, no convincing evidence of failure to respond to

auditory stimuli having their source located in the left

space has so far been provided. Right brain damaged

patients do show errors of localisation and lateralisa-

tion of auditory stimuli, shifting them towards the

right side'2 and sometimes answer a question ad-

dressed from the left turning their head and eyes to the

right,3 but they have never been reported to ignore a

stimulus located in^ the left space, except in response to double simultaneous stimulation. (^) Extinction, that (^) is,

the failure to perceive a stimulus in the contralateral

Address for reprint requests: Dr Ennio De Renzi, Clinica Neurologica, Via Del Pozzo 71, 1-41100 Modena, Italy.

Received 10 May 1988 and in revised (^) form 3 December 1988. Accepted 12 December 1988

ear when it is simultaneously delivered to the

ipsilateral ear, has been observed in nearly 50% of

unilaterally brain damaged patients in the early stage

of a stroke,4 but, contrary to what happens with visual

neglect, no significant association of the deficit with

right hemisphere lesions has been observed. This lack

of asymmetry in auditory extinction, which has been

confirmed in a recent investigation,5 casts some doubt

on the close relation of extinction to neglect.

Moreover, there^ were^ patients with^ long lasting extinc-

tion,4 whose injury was so located as to interrupt the

central auditory pathways in their course from the

medial geniculate body to the temporal cortex, thus

making extinction more likely to be contingent on a

sensory than an attentional impairment.

The difficulty of showing neglect in^ the auditory

modality is^ probably due^ to^ the^ physical characteris-

tics of acoustic stimuli and the (^) arrangement of the

neural apparatus transmitting them to the cortical

centres. The sound waves emitted by a left sided

acoustic source reach both ears, though for the right

one with^ delay and decreased intensity,6 and are thence

sent to both temporal cortices, though the contra-

lateral pathway is stronger. It follows that, when the

right hemisphere is damaged, the left hemisphere

remains able to perceive and attend to^ a^ sound^ coming

from the left side, due to its connections with both

ears, and can also lateralise it, though not accurately, by computing the time lag and the intensity difference with (^) which the right ear has been stimulated. It would appear that a prerequisite for bringing out auditory neglect, if it indeed (^) exists, is to confine the (^) acoustic stimulation to the ear contralateral to lesion. In this study we assessed the ability of brain- damaged patients with a recent (^) hemispheric disease (^) to monitor the interruptions that occurred at random intervals in one ear in a continuous sound delivered either unilaterally or bilaterally through earphones. We contrasted omissions in the ear contralateral and ipsilateral to the side of lesion and related them to the patients' manifestations of auditory extinction and visual neglect.

Material and methods

Subjects Fifty five patients participated in this investigation. Ten (^) were control (C) patients, hospitalised for diseases not (^) involving the brain (mean age: 56 4 yr), 15 were left (^) brain-damaged (LBD) patients (mean age: 61-2 yr) and 30 right brain- damaged (RBD) patients (mean age: 63-9 yr). Seven of the 15 LBD (^) patients were aphasic. The side of the lesion was based on clinical evidence and confirmed by CT in all patients, except two RBD patients who had negative CT findings, though showing unequivocal signs (left hemiparesis and hemianopia, respectively) of right hemisphere damage. In four RBD (^) patients CT also showed an old left vascular lesion that had been (^) clinically silent and in a fifth patient two silent left hemisphere (^) metastases in addition to a clinically evident metastasis of the right (^) hemisphere. Since neglect was the main subject of this investigation, the RBD (^) group selection was biased towards patients who presented clinically with signs of lateralised attentional disorders. Thus the (^) frequency with which neglect appears in the present sample cannot be generalised to the RBD population. Table 1 shows the clinical characteristics ofbrain-damaged patients.

Tests Patients were first submitted to pure tone audiometry to (^) rule out the presence ofmajor peripheral (^) deficits ofhearing and to ascertain whether there was an asymmetry between the (^) ear curves. If there was asymmetry the intensity of the (^) test signal delivered to the impaired ear was increased so as to equate the two (^) curves. It must be added that no patient with auditory neglect (see (^) below) showed a difference in threshold between ears.

De Renzi, Gentilini, Barbieri

Auditory attention test A sound generator delivered through (^) headphones to the patient a continuous 3000 Hz (^) pure tone at a level of 102 dB SPL; the sound lasted 4 minutes and was interrupted for 300 ms at intervals ranging from 8 to 20 ms. There were 24 interruptions, 12 per ear, to which the patient had to (^) respond by pressing the space bar ofa computer keyboard. The sound intensity could be adjusted independently in each channel in order to (^) compensate for hearing deficits, so that a (^) subjec- tively identical signal was provided to both ears in any case. The sound (^) generator was linked to an Apple II E computer by a^ versatile^ interface adaptor (6522). A computer program controlled the interruption pattern, specifically the 300 ms duration, the intervals between interruptions and the side of interruption in the binaural test. (^) Signal decay at interruption was (^) very fast, about 3 ms. The test (^) was presented in two versions, one^ mono-aural and^ the other binaural. In the mono-aural test the sound was first sent (^) to one ear until the 12 interruptions had occurred and then to the other ear, so that the patient knew where he had to focus attention. The order of ear presentation was pseudo-random. In the binaural test both ears received the sound, but the interruptions occurred quasi-randomly to either ear, with the restriction of no more than three consecutive interruptions to the same ear. In this case, therefore, the patient had to attend to both ears in order to perceive when the sound was interrupted. The following day the entire session was repeated and thus patients received (^48) signals per test, 24 in the right ear and 24 in the left ear. In the first session, patients were first familiarised with the procedure by the administration of a short version of either test with five 300 ms interruptions.

Auditory extinction Stimuli were delivered with two sound generators (75 dB), kept manually at a distance ofapproximately 10 cm from the ears, and (^) activated by pushing a button. Ten bilateral stimuli were administered, intermingled with 20 unilateral stimuli, 10 to the right and (^10) to the left ear. Responses were given by pointing with the forefinger to the ear (^) stimulated.

Visual neglect Two tests were given: (1) Reading test: The patient was (^) presented with a cardboard sheet, 41 cm x 29-5 cm, where the (^) sentence "La pera e caduta sotto l'albero" ("The pear has (^) fallen under the tree"), was written in 15 mm high (^) Letraset letters on a single line, 39 cm long. The patient was requested to read it aloud. Aphasics were encouraged to attempt reading, not (^) worrying about the errors they might make. If reading (^) proved to be impossible, they were requested to point to each word of (^) the sentence.

Table 1 Clinical characteristics ofbrain-damagedpatients

Aetiology Length ofillness* Parietalt Vascular Neoplastic (^) < 30 days > 30 days Visual neglect Aphasia involvement

RBD (^) patients (No: 30) 22 8 16 6 16 - (^15) LBD patients (^) (No: 15) 11 4 6 3 - (^7 )

*Vascular patients only. tBased on CT scan evidence.

Table 3 Clinical and (^) CTfindings in RBD patients with auditory neglect

De (^) Renzi, Gentilini, Barbieri

Pt. No. Aetiology V. Ext. T.Ext A. Ext (^) Visual neglect Locus oflesion

3 V nt nt 10/10 (^) severe R. inferior parietal infarct 4 V 10/10 nt 10/10 mild (^) R. thalamic-capsular haematoma 12 V 0/10 0/10 0/10 absent (^) R. inferior parietal + L. inferior parietal (old) infarct 15 V nt nt 4/10 severe (^) R. parietal haematoma + L. fronto-parietal (old) infarct 16 V (^) 10/10 10/10 2/10 severe (^) R. inferior parietal infarct 22 V nt 4/10 4/10 (^) severe R. thalamic-capsular infarct 23 V nt nt 9/10 severe (^) R. thalamic-capsular haematoma 28 N 10/10 10/10 0/10 absent (^) R. parietal + (^) L. parieto-occipital + L. occipital metastasis 30 V nt nt 10/10 severe (^) R. occipital and thalamic infarct

nt = not testable, because of severe somato- sensory or visual field defect. V. Ext.: visual extinction; T. Ext.: tactile extinction; A. Ext.: auditory extinction.

Discussion

The outcome of the present study provides clear

evidence that in RBD patients contralateral neglect

(that is, failure to detect a stimulus coming from the

left ear) extends to the auditory modality, provided

care is taken to confine the signal to one ear. Although

the phenomenon is already apparent in an attenuated

form when the patient focuses on the left ear, its

intensity is greatly enhanced in conditions of diffuse

attention, when the side of interruption is not known

in advance and the patient has to attend to the sound

of both ears. In this condition two RBD patients (No

23 and 30) missed practically all left ear stimuli and

two others (No 5 and 28) more than half. In three of

them (No 15, 23 and 30) omissions were still very

marked 2 to 3 months after the stroke. The length of

the testing session and the relatively rare occurrence of

interruptions have probably been instrumental in

favouring lapses in sustained attention, but the

prevalence of left ear omissions points to a specific,

lateralised deficit, which in the present series was

associated with damage to the right parietal lobe or

right thalamus, two structures known to play a pivotal

role in the circuit of lateralised attention.

It is noteworthy, however, that auditory neglect was

consistently associated neither with auditory extinc-

tion, nor with visual neglect. The finding of two

patients who had neglect, but not extinction disputes

the widely held view that extinction is but a minor

form of neglect. Taken in conjunction with compar-

able data found in the visual modality,5 and with the

three patients of the RBD group without neglect who

extinguished the majority of left ear stimuli, this

dissociation indicates that extinction does not neces-

sarily lie on a continuum of attentional disorders,

having neglect at its end, but may result from discrete

mechanisms and that any generalisation from extinc-

tion to neglect must be guarded.

The possibility first provided by the present

experiment to^ demonstrate the existence of auditory

neglect allowed us to compare its occurrence with that

of visual neglect and to gain information on the

organisation of^ the neural^ apparatus deploying atten-

tion to contralateral space. Though neglect has been

investigated mainly in the visual modality, current

theory78 tends to conceive of it as a multimodal

phenomenon, which affects the detection of con-

tralateral stimuli, whatever the sensory channel

through which they are conveyed. This may be so

because a lesion of the posterior parietal cortex, which

is the most frequent anatomical correlate of neglect in

humans91O tends to block the multiple unimodal (^) inputs

converging on it. Alternatively, it might be held that

this area is equipped to provide the neural template for

a superordinate surveyor of novel and significant

events occurring in contralateral space, the damage of

which results in failure to (^) attend to any type of stimuli

located therein. Such a hypothesis would imply that

neglect must necessarily involve all sensory inputs,

independently of the modality through which they are

transmitted, an assumption at variance with the

outcome of the present study, which (^) found nine RBD

patients with visual but not auditory neglect and two

RBD patients with auditory but not visual neglect.

These instances of dissociation are more compatible

with a (^) model envisaging a plurality of autonomous attentional control (^) systems decentralised at the level of

the single modalities, as argued by Bisiach et al " with

reference to anosognosia, and liable to produce

unimodal neglect.

So far we have focused on neglect affecting the

contralateral ear, but a failure to detect interruptions

was also apparent (^) in the ipsilateral ear of five RBD

patients, though never with the same severity as in the

contralateral ear. This finding cannot be disregarded

as an unspecific consequence of brain damage, since it

never occurred in LBD patients or in the RBD patients

without contralateral auditory neglect, even those who showed visual neglect. In two of the five patients there

was CT evidence of a previous clinically silent infarct

ofthe left hemisphere. Although a recent and clinically

manifest left brain lesion never (^) produces contralateral

neglect when occurring in isolation, the question may

Auditory neglect

be raised ofwhether an old lesion may^ play^ an^ additive effect when occurring in combination with^ a^ recent right-sided damage. On this account one would expect ipsilateral omissions to be more frequent following bilateral than unilateral (right) brain-damage, but table 2 shows that this was not the case. It is, therefore, legitimate to assume that in these^ patients,^ not^ differ- ing from patients with disease confined^ to^ the^ right hemisphere, right^ neglect^ depended^ on^ right brain damage and reflected the unique property^ of^ this hemisphere to also exert^ attentional control^ on^ the ipsilateral space. This^ hypothesis^ was^ advanced78^ to account for the much^ more^ frequent^ occurrence^ of neglect after right than^ left^ brain^ damage^ and^ found some support in^ normal studies,^ where^ EEG'2 and regional cerebral glucose metabolism data'3 suggested a right hemisphere participation^ in the^ processing^ of stimuli presented to the right space. There is also some pathological evidence^ pointing^ in^ the^ same^ direction. RBD patients have been found by Weintraub and Mesulam'4 to manifest, in addition to the classical left neglect syndrome, signs of inattention for the right space, where they missed some targets in a visual cancellation test. The interpretation of this finding is confounded by the fact that rightwards scanning unavoidably brings part of right-sided stimuli in the patient's left visual field. However, it receives some support by a study'5 showing that when single stimuli were flashed in different positions of the right visual field, those located to the left were responded to more slowly by right parietal patients. Weintraub and Mesulam's RBD patients were also found to be mildly, but significantly slower than controls on a^ tactile searching task when the target was in the^ right^ space, while no impairment in the left space was^ shown^ by LBD patients. This finding hints at a delayed^ attention in the tactical exploration of the^ ipsilateral space,^ but does not demonstrate a true neglect, comparable to that found in the contralateral space.'6 The^ right^ ear omissions, found in^ the present study, provide definite evidence for a substantial right hemisphere^ contribu- tion to the monitoring of^ right-located^ events,^ because the absence of exploratory movements in the auditory task avoids the discrepancy between body-centred space and^ eye-centred^ space^ entailed^ by^ visual^ tasks. What is surprising is that while left brain damage is apparently compensated by a well-functioning right hemisphere, the opposite is not always true.^ Is^ this asymmetry to be taken as^ evidence that^ right^ space surveillance is exerted more powerfully by the ipsilateral right hemisphere than the contralateral left hemisphere? More data are needed, particularly on^ the behaviour of LBD patients, before a^ conclusion^ can^ be reached. The auditory attention^ test^ appears^ to provide a good paradigm for^ pursuing^ this^ line^ of research.

We thank Dr^ C^ A^ Porro^ for his^ assistance^ in the

preparation of^ the computer^ program.^ Dr P^ Faglioni

for statistical advice and^ the Centro^ di^ Calcolo

dell'Universita di Modena^ for^ having provided^ its

facilities. This research was^ supported by^ a^ CNR grant and an MPI^ grant^ to^ Dr^ E^ De^ Renzi.

References

1 De Renzi E. Disorders of Space Exploration and Cogni- tion. Chichester: Wiley, 1982. 2 Bisiach E, Cornacchia L, Sterzi R, Vallar G. Disorders^ of perceived auditory lateralization after lesions of the right hemisphere. Brain 1984;107:37-52. 3 Altman JA, Balonov LJ, Deglin DL.^ Effects of unilateral disorder of the brain. Hemisphere function in^ man on directional hearing. Neuropsychologia 1979;17: 295-301. 4 De Renzi E, Gentilini M, Pattacini F.^ Auditory extinction following hemisphere damage. Neuropsychologia 1984;22:733-44. 5 Barbieri C, De Renzi E. Patterns of neglect^ dissociation. Behav Neurol (in press). 6 Blauert J. Spatial Hearing: the Psychophysics of^ Human Sound Localisation. Cambridge: The^ MIT^ Press,^ 1983. 7 Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein^ E.^ Neglect^ and related disorders. In:^ Heilman KM,^ Valenstein^ E,^ eds. Clinical Neuropsychology. New^ York:^ Oxford^ Univer- sity Press, 1985:243-93. 8 Mesulam MM.^ Attention,^ confusional^ states^ and^ neglect. In: Mesulam MM, ed.^ Principles^ of^ Behavioral Neurology. Contemporary^ neurology^ series,^ vol 26. Philadelphia: FA^ Davis,^ 1986:125-68. 9 Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E, Damasio AR. Localization of lesion in neglect. In: Kertesz A, ed. Localization in Neuropsychology. London: Academic Press, 1983:471-92. 10 Vallar G, Perani D. The anatomy of spatial neglect in humans. In: Jeannerod M, ed. Neurophysiological and Neuropsychological Aspects of Spatial Neglect. Advan- ces in Psychology, vol 45. Amsterdam: North Holland, 1986:235-58. 11 Bisiach E, Vallar G, Perani D, Papagno C, Berti^ A. Unawareness of disease following lesions of the^ right hemisphere: Anosognosia for^ hemiplegia and^ anosog-

nosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia 1986;24:

471-82. 12 Heilman KM, Van Den^ Abell^ T.^ Right^ hemisphere dominance for attention: The mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology 1980;30:327-30. 13 Reivich M, Gur RC, Alavi A. Positron emission tomographic studies of sensory stimulation, cognitive processes and anxiety. Hum Neurobiol 1983;2:25-33. 14 Weintraub S, Mesulam MM.^ Right cerebral^ dominance in (^) spatial attention. Further evidence based on spatial neglect. Arch Neurol^ 1987;44:621-5. 15 Ladavas E. Is the hemispatial deficit produced by right parietal lobe damage associated with^ retinal^ or gravitational coordinate?^ Brain^ 1987;110:^ 167-80. 16 De Renzi E, Faglioni P, Scotti^ G.^ Hemispheric^ contribu- tion to (^) exploration of space through the^ visual and tactile modality. Cortx 1970;6:191-203.